翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Conspiracies (novel)
・ Conspiracies (TV series)
・ Conspiracies (video game)
・ Conspiracies against the laity
・ Conspiracy
・ Conspiracy (1927 film)
・ Conspiracy (1930 film)
・ Conspiracy (1939 film)
・ Conspiracy (2000 film)
・ Conspiracy (2001 film)
・ Conspiracy (2008 film)
・ Consonant
・ Consonant (band)
・ Consonant cluster
・ Consonant gradation
Consonant harmony
・ Consonant mutation
・ Consonant voicing and devoicing
・ Consonus
・ Consorci de la Zona Franca
・ Consorcio ARA
・ Consorcio de Compensacion de Seguros
・ Consorcio Regional de Transportes de Madrid
・ Consorms
・ Consors
・ Consort
・ Consort (nautical)
・ Consort Airport
・ Consort Ban
・ Consort beagles


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Consonant harmony : ウィキペディア英語版
Consonant harmony

Consonant harmony is a type of "long-distance" phonological assimilation akin to the similar assimilatory process involving vowels, i.e. vowel harmony.
==Examples==
A good discussion of consonant harmony typology is found in Rose and Walker's 2004 paper in the journal ''Language''. "A Typology of Consonant Agreement as Correspondence."
One of the more common harmony processes is ''coronal harmony''. This type of harmony affects the coronal fricatives, such as ''s'' and ''sh'' in a word, requiring all the coronal fricatives in the word to belong either to the +anterior class (''s''-like sounds) or the -anterior class (''sh''-like sounds). Such patterns are found in the Dene (Athabaskan) languages such as Navajo (Young and Morgan 1987, McDonough 2003), Tahltan (Shaw 1991), Western Apache, and in Chumash on the California coast (Applegate 1972, Campbell 1997), to name a few examples. In Tahltan, Shaw shows that the coronal harmony affects three coronal fricatives, ''s'', ''sh'' and the interdental ''th''. The following examples are given by de Reuse: in Western Apache, the verbal prefix ''si-'' is an alveolar fricative, as in the following forms:
(unicode:
* ''siką̄ą̄'' "a container and its contents are in position"
* ''sitłēēd'' "mushy matter is in position"
* ''siyį̄į̄'' "a load/pack/burden is in position"
* ''sinéʼ'' "three or more flexible objects are in position"
* ''siłāā'' "a slender flexible object is in position"
* ''siʼą̄ą̄'' "a solid roundish object is in position"
* ''sitsooz'' "a flat flexible object is in position"
* ''siziid'' "liquid matter is in position")
However, when this prefix ''si-'' occurs before a verb stem that contains a post-alveolar affricate, the ''si-'' surfaces as the post-alveolar ''shi-'':
* ''shijaa'' "three or more solid rigid inanimate objects are in position"
Thus all the sibilant obstruents (fricatives and affricates) in these languages are divided into two groups, +anterior (''s'', ''ts'', ''dz'') and -anterior (''sh'', ''ch'', ''j''). In Navajo, as in most languages with consonant harmony, there is a constraint on the shape of roots (a well-formedness constraint) that is identical to the harmony process. Thus all roots with sibiliant affricates or fricatives have the same value for anteriority. Shaw (1991) provides a phonological analysis of this process, using data from research on Tahltan language.
There are two interesting aspects of this process in Navajo. First, morphemes that participate are domain specific, only the two rightmost domains are affected (conjunct + stem). Verbal morphemes from the outer or 'disjunct' domain are not affected by the process; i.e. the process is morphologically conditioned. Second, the lateral affricate and fricative (''dl'', ''tł'' and ''ł'') appear with both values. Young and Morgan (1987) offer an extensive sets of examples of this type of morpheme alternation in Navajo.
A different example of coronal harmony occurs in Sanskrit, where () is retroflexed to if certain consonants precede it in the same word, even at a distance.
Various Austronesian languages exhibit consonant harmony among the liquid consonants, with () assimilating at a distance to () or vice versa.
Guaraní shows nasal harmony, by which certain affixes have alternative forms according to whether the root includes a nasal (vowel or consonant) or not. For instance, the reflexive prefix is realized as oral ''je-'' when preceding an oral stem like ''juka'' "kill", but as nasal ''ñe-'' when preceding a nasal stem like ''nupã'' "hit", where the ã makes the stem nasal.
Some Finnish speakers find it hard to pronounce both 'b' and 'p' in foreign words (e.g. ''pubi''), so they voice (''bubi'') or devoice (''pupi'') the entire word. It should however be noted that the distinction between these consonants is not native to Finnish.〔Finnish does have one native voiced stop, /d/, but it is not subject to similar behavior, e.g. ''tädit'' ("aunts"). This may be because the distinction between and involves the place of articulation (dental vs. alveolar) in addition to voice.〕 Native Finnish words do not use the letter 'b'.

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Consonant harmony」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.